Brain Activity Changes During Rem Sleep: A Mystery Unveiled

which brain areas show activity changes in rem sleep

REM sleep is associated with distinct global cortical dynamics and is controlled by the occipital cortex. During REM sleep, the brain is highly active, with brain waves similar to those seen during wakefulness. The cerebral cortex is spontaneously active during sleep, and the occipital cortex is particularly active during REM sleep.

REM sleep is characterised by rapid eye movements, irregular breathing, and a temporary loss of muscle tone. The brainstem plays a key role in regulating REM sleep, with the pons, medulla, and midbrain controlling the transitions between wake and sleep. The thalamus and basal forebrain also promote sleep and wakefulness.

The brain's activity during REM sleep is associated with dreaming, memory consolidation, emotional processing, and brain development. Dreaming, which is more vivid during REM sleep, may aid in emotional processing. Memory consolidation, which occurs during both REM and non-REM sleep, involves the processing of new learnings and the strengthening of some memories. Emotional processing is facilitated by the activation of the amygdala during REM sleep. Brain development may also be promoted by REM sleep, as newborns spend a significant amount of their sleep time in this stage.

The functions and regulation of REM sleep are not yet fully understood, and further research is needed to elucidate its role in emotional reactivation and other processes.

Characteristics Values
Brain areas showing activity changes in REM sleep Occipital cortex, brainstem, thalamus, limbic areas, temporo-occipital cortices, prefrontal areas, amygdala, hippocampus, anterior cingulate cortex, basal forebrain, striatum, insula, posterior cingulate gyrus, cerebellum, pons, medulla, midbrain, basal ganglia, basal forebrain, caudate nucleus, amygdala, hippocampus, anterior cingulate cortex, parahippocampal gyrus, putamen, premotor and supplementary areas, dorsomedial PFC, right hippocampus, right amygdaloid complex, striatum, insula, posterior cingulate gyrus, ponto-mesencephalic tegmentum, cerebellum, hippocampus, parahippocampal gyrus, prefrontal cortex, hippocampus, MPFC, vMPFC, striatum, insula, precuneus, and posterior cingulate gyrus
REM sleep characteristics Low-amplitude synchronization of fast oscillations in the cortical EEG, very low muscle tone (atonia) in the EMG, singlets and clusters of REMs in the EOG, myoclonic twitches, pronounced fluctuations in cardio-respiratory rhythms and core body temperature, penile erection and clitoral tumescence, theta rhythm in the hippocampal EEG, spiky field potentials in the pons, lateral geniculate nucleus, and occipital cortex, occurrence of vivid dreaming, REM sleep muscle atonia, rapid eye movements, irregular breathing, a rise in heart rate, and the ability to be awoken more easily than during non-REM sleep

shunsleep

The role of the thalamus and brainstem in REM sleep

The thalamus, the brainstem, and the cerebral cortex are all involved in the process of REM sleep. The thalamus sends and receives information from the senses to the cerebral cortex. During most stages of sleep, the thalamus is quiet, but during REM sleep, it is active, sending the cortex images, sounds, and sensations that fill our dreams.

The brainstem, which is made up of the pons, medulla, and midbrain, controls the transitions between being awake and asleep. It also plays a role in REM sleep, sending signals to relax the muscles so that we don't act out our dreams.

The cerebral cortex is the covering of the brain and is active during sleep. It has many functions, including interpreting and processing short- and long-term memory.

The thalamus and brainstem, therefore, play critical roles in REM sleep. The thalamus provides the sensory information that the cerebral cortex uses to generate dreams, while the brainstem ensures that we remain relaxed and don't act out these dreams.

shunsleep

The importance of the amygdala and hippocampus during REM sleep

The amygdala and hippocampus are two brain regions that show activity changes during REM sleep, with the amygdala responding to social exclusion and rejection, and the hippocampus playing a role in memory consolidation.

The Amygdala During REM Sleep

During REM sleep, the amygdala shows increased activity in response to social exclusion and rejection, with a greater response when individuals are passively enduring social exclusion rather than actively regulating their emotions. This increased amygdala activity is thought to signal the relevance of information to the individual's social well-being.

Selective REM sleep suppression has been found to increase general negative affect and enhance amygdala responses, with a stronger effect on negative affect in the morning after a night of sleep. However, it is important to note that the effect of REM sleep suppression on self-reported emotional responses to social exclusion has not been consistently observed across studies.

The Hippocampus During REM Sleep

The hippocampus plays a crucial role in memory formation and consolidation. During REM sleep, the hippocampus exhibits slow oscillations, which are thought to facilitate the integration of new information into existing cortical networks. This process is believed to contribute to memory consolidation during sleep.

In summary, the amygdala and hippocampus are two brain regions that show activity changes during REM sleep, with the amygdala responding to social exclusion and rejection, and the hippocampus playing a role in memory consolidation.

shunsleep

The relationship between REM sleep and dreaming

Dreaming and REM sleep are controlled by different brain mechanisms. Dreaming is controlled by forebrain mechanisms, while REM sleep is controlled by the brainstem.

REM sleep is the fourth stage of sleep and is characterised by rapid eye movement, irregular breathing, elevated heart rate, and increased brain activity. It is also referred to as active sleep, desynchronized sleep, paradoxical sleep, rhombencephalic sleep, and dream sleep.

During REM sleep, the brain is highly active and dreams typically occur. The brain's activity during this stage is similar to its activity when awake.

REM sleep is important for brain development, memory consolidation, emotional processing, and learning. It is also when the brain repairs itself and processes emotional experiences.

The majority of dreams occur during REM sleep. However, dreams can also occur during non-REM sleep. Dreams during REM sleep are usually more vivid than those during non-REM sleep.

The amygdala, the part of the brain that processes emotions, is activated during REM sleep. This suggests that dreaming may help with emotional processing.

While the brain is active during REM sleep, the body is temporarily paralysed to prevent acting out dreams.

Does Cannabis Affect Your REM Sleep?

You may want to see also

shunsleep

The role of the pons in REM sleep

The pons is a small area in the brainstem that plays a crucial role in regulating REM sleep. It contains groups of nerve cells that control sleep and wakefulness. During REM sleep, the pons sends signals to relax muscles, which are essential for body posture and limb movements, preventing people from acting out their dreams.

The pons is also involved in generating the distinctive neurophysiological features of REM sleep, such as rapid eye movements, muscle atonia, and ponto-geniculo-occipital (PGO) waves. PGO waves are spike-like electrical signals that occur during REM sleep and are believed to play a role in memory consolidation.

The pons contains a population of neurons that express corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH), which has been found to regulate both REM sleep and P-waves. Optogenetic and chemogenetic experiments have shown that activation of these CRH neurons promotes REM sleep and increases the frequency of P-waves, while inhibition of this population reduces REM sleep and P-wave frequency.

The CRH neurons in the pons project to the subcoeruleus nucleus of the pons, which is critical for generating P-waves, and the nucleus incertus, which is involved in modulating the hippocampal theta rhythm that characterises REM sleep. The exact mechanisms by which the pons regulates REM sleep and P-waves are still being elucidated, but it is clear that this small brain region plays a crucial role in controlling these aspects of sleep.

shunsleep

The effects of REM sleep on memory consolidation

Sleep is an important part of our daily routine, with humans spending about a third of their time doing it. Quality sleep is essential for survival, just like food and water. Sleep is important to a number of brain functions, including how nerve cells (neurons) communicate with each other. The brain and body remain remarkably active while we sleep. Recent findings suggest that sleep plays a housekeeping role in removing toxins in the brain that build up while we are awake.

REM sleep is characterised by a constellation of events, including:

  • Low-amplitude synchronisation of fast oscillations in the cortical EEG (also called activated EEG)
  • Very low muscle tone (atonia) in the EMG. The atonia is observed to be particularly strong on antigravity muscles, whereas the diaphragm and extra-ocular muscles retain substantial tone
  • Singlets and clusters of REMs in the EOG

REM sleep is also associated with an intense neuronal activity, similar to waking levels. Brain glucose metabolism and oxygen utilization are elevated during REM sleep and reach levels comparable to wakefulness. However, the spatial distribution of brain activity during REM sleep and wakefulness differ considerably.

REM sleep and memory consolidation

The idea that REM sleep duration should increase with learning is based on the hypothesis that increased learning will require increased memory consolidation and hence more REM sleep time. However, it is unclear whether such a manipulation consistently increases the total amount of learning that occurs. One can assume that an animal is continuously learning, albeit not at the behest of the experimenter. There is no guarantee that the novelty of a new experimental situation will produce a substantial overall increase in learning, unless one assumes that minimal learning occurs in the home cage situation where the animal interacts with its conspecifics and others who handle it, anticipates food and water changes, and responds to sensory stimuli.

Even if the novelty of the learning situation is assumed to produce a marked increase in the quantity of learning, it will not produce this effect alone. It is quite likely that stress associated with shock avoidance (used in many REM sleep–learning studies), frustration involved in appetitive reinforcement paradigms, and other emotional aspects of the situation will have a major impact on the animal. The assumption that levels of stress are not correlated (positively or negatively) with the nature of the learning task and with the animal’s success at the task is unproven and unexplored in most of these studies. This issue is particularly worrisome because it has been shown that moderate stress, in the absence of any imposed learning task, can produce a marked increase in REM sleep, whereas higher levels of stress disrupt sleep. The inability to measure and separate stress and other emotional variables readily from learning makes it difficult to determine which of these, if any, are affecting subsequent REM sleep.

Some studies have shown that REM sleep deprivation blocks consolidation, whereas others reported no effect of the procedure; still, others reported improved consolidation with REM deprivation. The failure of deprivation to prevent consolidation has been attributed to the nature of the task, with some authors concluding that only more complex tasks require REM sleep for consolidation. However, inspection of the literature reveals that experimental results varied even when the same task was assigned. For example, REM sleep deprivation has been shown to block recall of “shuttle box avoidance” tasks in some studies but not in others. One explanation offered for this variability has been the “REM sleep window” hypothesis discussed above. Most studies have used REM sleep deprivation immediately after learning a task, including those with positive as well as negative results. Other studies have claimed better results if one waits for a REM sleep window, although even in this situation, both positive and negative results have occurred.

A less stressful REM sleep deprivation technique was devised in which a gentle rocking motion was used to prevent REM sleep in rats. With this deprivation procedure, no learning deficit was seen on the same task that had been disrupted by the platform deprivation technique. This result suggests that stress, rather than REM sleep loss, was the critical variable.

Human REM sleep deprivation can be accomplished with polygraphic monitoring by awakening the subject whenever REM sleep begins. REM sleep deprivation results in more frequent attempts to enter REM sleep, but deprivation can be accomplished with as few as nine awakenings per night. Because in humans most REM sleep time occurs late in the sleep period, some human studies have REM or control non-REM sleep deprivation effects confounded with the circadian time of deprivation.

The existing literature does not indicate a major role for REM sleep in memory consolidation. However, sleep disruption occurring before learning will affect performance in learning tasks. This disruption is not due to the loss of sleep per se, but rather to the intrusions of sleep into waking during the learning task. In a similar way, sleep loss, because of the resulting impairment of concentration and sleep intrusions, will interfere with recall. Just as nutritional status, ambient temperature, level of stress, blood oxygenation, and other variables clearly affect the ability to learn, adequate sleep is vital for optimal performance in learning tasks.

Frequently asked questions

REM sleep is the fourth out of four total stages of sleep. It is characterised by relaxed muscles, quick eye movement, irregular breathing, elevated heart rate, and increased brain activity.

During REM sleep, your eyes move rapidly behind your closed eyes, your heart rate speeds up, and your breathing becomes irregular. In contrast to other stages of sleep, in which your brain waves slow down, your brain is highly active during REM sleep, and your brain waves become more variable.

You experience your first cycle of REM sleep about 60 to 90 minutes after falling asleep. As part of a full night's sleep, you cycle through four stages of sleep multiple times: three stages of non-REM sleep, followed by one stage of REM sleep. Each cycle through all the sleep stages takes 90 to 120 minutes to complete. With each new cycle, you spend increasing amounts of time in REM sleep, with most of your REM sleep taking place in the second half of the night.

All sleep is important, but REM sleep in particular plays an important role in dreaming, memory, emotional processing, and healthy brain development.

We need the most REM sleep as infants and children, when our brains are still developing. Newborn babies spend eight hours in REM sleep each day. By adulthood, we only need an average of two hours of REM sleep each night.

Multiple studies of both humans and animals suggest that being deprived of REM sleep interferes with memory formation. However, memory problems associated with a loss of REM sleep could be due to overall sleep disruption, since those often occur together.

Written by
Reviewed by
Share this post
Print
Did this article help you?

Leave a comment